[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Linear Latent Growth Curve Model Examining the Association Between Working Memory (WM) Development and Crashes
Linear Latent Growth Curve Model Examining the Association Between Working Memory (WM) Development and Crashes

Model 1 showing the association between the slope of the WM growth trajectory and crashes. Values at arrows beside latent WM intercept and slope are estimates of variance and values at arrows from factors to crashes indicate parameter estimates. e indicates residual variances of the working memory score at each wave.

aSignificant parameter values (P < .05).

Figure 2.  Working Memory (WM) Composite Scores at Each Wave of Assessment
Working Memory (WM) Composite Scores at Each Wave of Assessment

Relative changes in working memory from wave 2 to wave 6 assessment points for drivers with a crash history (n = 25), drivers with no crash history (n = 59), and nondrivers who responded to the follow-up survey (n = 34).

Table 1.  Demographic Profile of Young Driver Sample
Demographic Profile of Young Driver Sample
Table 2.  Philadelphia Trajectory Study Measure Values From the Final Wave (Wave 6)
Philadelphia Trajectory Study Measure Values From the Final Wave (Wave 6)
Table 3.  Pearson Correlation Coefficients (r) Between the Individual Characteristic Variables Recorded at Wave 6 and the Follow-up Driving Survey Outcomes
Pearson Correlation Coefficients (r) Between the Individual Characteristic Variables Recorded at Wave 6 and the Follow-up Driving Survey Outcomes
1.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Leading causes of death reports, national and regional: 1999–2014. http://webappa.cdc.gov/sasweb/ncipc/leadcaus10_us.html. Published 2015. Accessed March 1, 2019.
2.
Tefft  BC; AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. Rates of motor vehicle crashes, injuries and deaths in relation to driver age, United States, 2014-2015. https://aaafoundation.org/rates-motor-vehicle-crashes-injuries-deaths-relation-driver-age-united-states-2014-2015/. Published June 2017. Accessed April 3, 2019.
3.
Curry  AE, Pfeiffer  MR, Durbin  DR, Elliott  MR.  Young driver crash rates by licensing age, driving experience, and license phase.  Accid Anal Prev. 2015;80:243-250. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2015.04.019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Gershon  P, Ehsani  JP, Zhu  C,  et al.  Crash risk and risky driving behavior among adolescents during learner and independent driving periods.  J Adolesc Health. 2018;63(5):568-574. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.04.012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Walshe  EA, Ward McIntosh  C, Romer  D, Winston  FK.  Executive function capacities, negative driving behavior and crashes in young drivers.  Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14(11):1314. doi:10.3390/ijerph14111314PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Cowan  N.  Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education.  Educ Psychol Rev. 2014;26(2):197-223. doi:10.1007/s10648-013-9246-yPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Engle  RW, Kane  MJ.  Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control.  Psychol Learn Motiv. 2004;44:145-200. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44005-XGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Isbell  E, Fukuda  K, Neville  HJ, Vogel  EK.  Visual working memory continues to develop through adolescence.  Front Psychol. 2015;6:696. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00696PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Luna  B, Padmanabhan  A, O’Hearn  K.  What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence?  Brain Cogn. 2010;72(1):101-113. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Satterthwaite  TD, Wolf  DH, Erus  G,  et al.  Functional maturation of the executive system during adolescence.  J Neurosci. 2013;33(41):16249-16261. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2345-13.2013PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Khurana  A, Romer  D, Betancourt  LM, Brodsky  NL, Giannetta  JM, Hurt  H.  Working memory ability predicts trajectories of early alcohol use in adolescents: the mediational role of impulsivity.  Addiction. 2013;108(3):506-515. doi:10.1111/add.12001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Conklin  HM, Luciana  M, Hooper  CJ, Yarger  RS.  Working memory performance in typically developing children and adolescents: behavioral evidence of protracted frontal lobe development.  Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;31(1):103-128. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn3101_6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Baddeley  A.  Working memory: theories, models, and controversies.  Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:1-29. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Diamond  A.  Executive functions.  Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135-168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Groeger  JA.  Understanding Driving: Applying Cognitive Psychology to a Complex Everyday Task. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2000.
16.
Romer  D, Lee  YC, McDonald  CC, Winston  FK.  Adolescence, attention allocation, and driving safety.  J Adolesc Health. 2014;54(5)(suppl):S6-S15. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.10.202PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Haith  AM, Krakauer  JW.  The multiple effects of practice: skill, habit and reduced cognitive load.  Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018;20:196-201. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Mäntylä  T, Karlsson  MJ, Marklund  M.  Executive control functions in simulated driving.  Appl Neuropsychol. 2009;16(1):11-18. doi:10.1080/09084280802644086PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Pope  CN, Ross  LA, Stavrinos  D.  Association between executive function and problematic adolescent driving.  J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2016;37(9):702-711. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000353PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Starkey  NJ, Isler  RB.  The role of executive function, personality and attitudes to risks in explaining self-reported driving behavior in adolescent and adult male drivers.  Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav. 2016;38:127-136. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2016.01.013Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Ross  V, Jongen  E, Brijs  T, Ruiter  R, Brijs  K, Wets  G.  The relation between cognitive control and risky driving in young novice drivers.  Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2015;22(1):61-72. doi:10.1080/23279095.2013.838958PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Ross  V, Jongen  EM, Wang  W,  et al.  Investigating the influence of working memory capacity when driving behavior is combined with cognitive load: an LCT study of young novice drivers.  Accid Anal Prev. 2014;62:377-387. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2013.06.032PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Romer  D, Betancourt  L, Giannetta  JM, Brodsky  NL, Farah  M, Hurt  H.  Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents.  Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(13):2916-2926. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Khurana  A, Romer  D, Betancourt  LM, Hurt  H.  Modeling trajectories of sensation seeking and impulsivity dimensions from early to late adolescence: universal trends or distinct sub-groups?  J Youth Adolesc. 2018;47(9):1992-2005. doi:10.1007/s10964-018-0891-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Alderman  EM, Johnston  BD; Committee on Adolescence; Council on Injury, Violence, and Poison Prevention.  The teen driver.  Pediatrics. 2018;142(4):e20182163. doi:10.1542/peds.2018-2163PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Reason  J, Manstead  A, Stradling  S, Baxter  J, Campbell  K.  Errors and violations on the roads: a real distinction?  Ergonomics. 1990;33(10-11):1315-1332. doi:10.1080/00140139008925335PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Walshe  EA, Winston  FK, Betancourt  LM, Arena  K, Romer  D.  Cell phone use while driving: a pattern of risk in young novice drivers.  Acad J Pediatr Neonatology. 2018;6(4). doi:10.19080/AJPN.2018.06.555752Google Scholar
28.
Curry  AE, Metzger  KB, Pfeiffer  MR, Elliott  MR, Winston  FK, Power  TJ.  Motor vehicle crash risk among adolescents and young adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171(8):756-763. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.0910PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Classen  S, Monahan  M, Brown  KE, Hernandez  S.  Driving indicators in teens with attention deficit hyperactivity and/or autism spectrum disorder.  Can J Occup Ther. 2013;80(5):274-283. doi:10.1177/0008417413501072PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Fischer  M, Barkley  RA, Smallish  L, Fletcher  K.  Hyperactive children as young adults: driving abilities, safe driving behavior, and adverse driving outcomes.  Accid Anal Prev. 2007;39(1):94-105. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2006.06.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Isen  JD, Sparks  JC, Iacono  WG.  Predictive validity of delay discounting behavior in adolescence: a longitudinal twin study.  Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;22(5):434-443. doi:10.1037/a0037340PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
White  HR, Beardslee  J, Pardini  D.  Early predictors of maturing out of marijuana use among young men.  Addict Behav. 2017;65:56-62. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.09.007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Romer  D, Betancourt  LM, Brodsky  NL, Giannetta  JM, Yang  W, Hurt  H.  Does adolescent risk taking imply weak executive function? a prospective study of relations between working memory performance, impulsivity, and risk taking in early adolescence.  Dev Sci. 2011;14(5):1119-1133. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01061.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
Pediatrics
September 13, 2019

青年驾驶员工作记忆发展与车祸

Author Affiliations
  • 1The Annenberg Public Policy Center at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia
  • 2The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
  • 3College of Education, University of Oregon, Eugene
JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(9):e1911421. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.11421
关键点 español English

问题  青年驾驶员工作记忆发展的变化是否与车祸有关?

结果  在本项针对 84 名社区青年驾驶员的队列研究中,当控制其他车祸危险因素时,工作记忆增长(斜率,而非基线(截距))的相对变化与报告车祸呈负相关。

意义  对青少年工作记忆发展的常规评估可能有助于识别出具有相对不同的工作记忆增长轨迹的青少年,这些人更有可能卷入车祸。

Abstract

Importance  Adolescent drivers have the highest rate of motor vehicle crashes, and among equally novice drivers, crash risk is inversely age graded. Working memory (WM), crucial to driving hazard awareness, is also age graded, with ongoing development into late adolescence. Variability in WM capacity and growth trajectory positions WM as a candidate crash risk factor for study, clinical screening, and possible preventative intervention.

Objective  To test the association between crashes and differential WM development.

Design, Setting, and Participants  This prospective cohort study used data from a longitudinal cohort of 118 community youth in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. Working memory and other risk factors were measured annually from age 11 to 13 years (prelicensure, in 2005) to 14 to 16 years (in 2009), and again at 18 to 20 years (in 2013). In 2015, a follow-up survey of driving experience identified 84 participants who had started driving. Latent growth curve modeling was used to examine the association between variability in the baseline (intercept) and developmental trajectory (slope) of WM and the crash outcome.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Self-reported crashes were the primary outcome. Variability in the relative growth of WM development along with traits and behaviors associated with risky driving were assessed.

Results  Of 84 participants (39 [46%] male; mean [SD] age, 20.46 [1.09] years), 25 (29.8%) reported they had been involved in at least 1 crash. Controlling for other crash risk factors, the model indicated that variation in the linear slope of WM growth was inversely associated with reporting a crash (b = −6.41; SE = 2.64; P = .02). Crashes were also associated with reckless driving behavior (b = 0.40; SE = 0.18; P = .03). Variation in the intercept of WM was not associated with crashes (b = −0.245; SE = 0.67; P = .72).

Conclusions and Relevance  The results suggest that a relatively slower WM growth trajectory is associated with young driver crashes. Routine assessment of WM across adolescence may help to identify at-risk teen drivers and opportunities for providing adaptive interventions (eg, driving aids or training) that can address limitations in WM-related skills that are critical for safe driving.

Introduction

Driving, a skill commonly acquired during adolescence, is also a potentially dangerous behavior. Motor vehicle crashes are the leading cause of injury and death for adolescents in the United States.1 When compared with other age groups, adolescent drivers have the highest rate of crashes, injuries, and mortality.2 Poor skills and inexperience explain some of this risk early after licensure: crash rates are highest during the first 6 months of independent driving.3,4 However, even among equally novice drivers, crash risk is age graded. At the outset of independent driving, drivers aged approximately 17 years have a higher crash rate than drivers aged approximately 20 years.3 Given this age-graded risk, individual variation in development is likely associated with crash risk, but its nature and contribution to this risk is not known.

One possible developmental source of crash risk is working memory (WM),5 which is associated with learning and other risk outcomes in adolescents.6,7 Working memory has a protracted development into late adolescence,8,9 with intraindividual and interindividual variability,10 but generally demonstrates a linear growth trajectory during adolescence.11,12 This limited-capacity system allows for in-the-moment monitoring, updating, and planning, which are important for decision making, managing complex tasks, and controlling attention in the face of distraction.13,14 Given that driving is a complex task requiring attentional and related abilities (important for situational awareness), variability in WM development may contribute to crashes during adolescence. For example, safe driving involves scanning, monitoring, and updating information about the vehicle and environment while managing multiple subtasks (eg, adjusting speed, steering, in-vehicle controls) and distractors (eg, peer passengers and mobile phones).15,16 Working memory development may be even more critical among novice drivers who have not yet automated the subtasks of driving. During the learning phase, adequate cognitive capacity is required to develop both the declarative and procedural memory required for safe driving.17 Therefore, if some adolescents start driving before their WM capacity has sufficiently developed, they may be at increased risk for crashes for this reason alone.

Indeed, previous studies have shown that in typically developing teens, lower WM capacity is associated with more reckless and inattentive driving, crashes, and poorer performance on simulated driving tasks.18-21 In addition, driving performance deteriorates when young drivers complete a secondary WM task while driving in a simulator, but is less affected in those with a higher baseline WM capacity.22 Furthermore, variability in WM has been associated with impulsivity and substance use during adolescence,23,24 known risk factors for reckless driving. However, prior studies were cross-sectional, often focused on driving behavior rather than crash outcomes, and rarely controlled for other known risk factors (eg, impulsivity, substance use). To our knowledge, no studies to date have longitudinally examined WM development and its association with crashes by concurrently investigating WM with personality traits and unsafe behaviors.

Young driver policy statements25 outline the importance of considering development when pediatricians counsel families about driving, pointing to recommendations for atypical development (eg, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD]) as a risk factor. To our knowledge, no evidence exists for recommendations around expected individual variation in typical neurocognitive development, including WM capacity, which co-occurs during the period of learning to drive and early licensure. Therefore, this study aims to enhance the scientific foundation for young driver policies with an initial investigation of individual variation in the trajectory of WM development in association with unsafe driving and crash risk. The question that arises for public policy and pediatric care is whether differential development in WM places some adolescents at risk of crashes due to limitations in attention and related processing skills that are critical for safe driving. This study addresses 2 gaps in the field by (1) assessing the independent prospective association of WM development with young driver crashes while (2) controlling for other risk-related traits and behaviors (impulsivity, sensation seeking, reckless driving, and substance use). We do this using data from a longitudinal trajectory study of community youth that measured WM development from early to late adolescence, as well as the associated risk-related traits and behaviors, and examine the association between these factors and crashes reported in a follow-up survey after a year or more of independent driving.

Methods
Participants

Participants were recruited from a larger longitudinal cohort of community youth in Philadelphia, Pennsylvania, the Philadelphia Trajectory Study (PTS).11 The original longitudinal study enrolled a community sample of 387 youths aged 10 to 12 years who were assessed annually across 5 waves from 2004 to 2010, and again in 2013 and 2014 at the sixth and final wave of testing, retaining 290 participants (aged 18-20 years). We invited all participants retained at wave 6 to take part in a follow-up survey of driving experiences conducted in 2015. One hundred eighteen participants responded and provided written consent. Of these, 84 participants held a driver’s license and were included in the following analysis. As a check for sample bias, the characteristics of the driver sample and the larger PTS sample were compared.

Study Design

A survey of driving experience was conducted in 2015 as follow-up to the PTS study. Using latent growth curve analysis (LGC), we examined the developmental trajectory of WM across adolescence using waves 2 to 6 (2006-2012) owing to the addition of an object n-back task to the WM battery after wave 1. Only 1 participant was missing WM data at wave 3; otherwise, all other variables in the analysis were complete. We examined the association between the latent intercept and slope of the LGC model and crashes at the follow-up, along with other factors of driving behavior, risk-related traits, and behavior. This study was approved by the institutional review board of the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia. Participants provided written informed consent. This study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline for cohort studies.

Measures
Longitudinal Measures

The longitudinal PTS measured WM on 4 tasks and a number of risk traits and behaviors, including impulsivity, sensation seeking, delay discounting, and incidences of fighting behavior and substance use dependency. These measures have been described previously.11 A principal component analysis using data from wave 5 and 6 for the total sample of 118 participants revealed that the 2-back, Corsi-block tapping, and spatial WM tasks loaded on 1 factor (explaining 46% of the variance, Cronbach α = 0.83), while the backward digit span loaded on a separate factor alone (explaining 15% of the variance). The 2-back, Corsi-block tapping, and spatial WM scores were standardized to derive a composite score of WM at each wave (Cronbach α ranged from 0.67 to 0.73). This score measured the relative standing of each adolescent in the sample at each wave of the study.

A principal component analysis was used to reduce the risk traits and behaviors to common latent factor scores for use in the LGC model. A risk behavior score derived from wave 6 assessments comprised acting without thinking (AWT) impulsivity, sensation seeking, fighting behavior, alcohol use dependence, and marijuana use dependence. This factor was used in the LGC model to represent this set of risk behaviors near the time of independent driving.

Driving Survey

This self-report survey included items adapted from the commonly used Driver Behavior Questionnaire26 to gauge license experience (eg, type of license, number of years driving), self-reported history of crashes, and how often participants engaged in reckless and unsafe driving behaviors. Reason et al26 noted that items contributing to a factor of reckless driving (intentionally deviating from safe driving by speeding and running traffic lights) were strongly associated with crashes.

Nine high-risk (“definite risk to others”) items were selected from the Driver Behavior Questionnaire to measure engagement in intentional reckless driving (eg, running traffic lights, unsafe overtaking, and ignoring speed limits) and driving errors (eg, failing to check mirrors when pulling out and misjudging the speed of an oncoming vehicle). Given trends of increasing mobile phone use while driving, we added 2 additional items to measure engagement in texting and calls while driving. Participants indicated how often they engaged in each behavior using a 6-point scale (1 = never to 6 = nearly all the time). Many of these items were significantly associated (including the additional mobile phone behavior items) and revealed 2 latent components reflecting intentional reckless driving (including mobile phone use) (Cronbach α = 0.81) and driving errors (Cronbach α = 0.60) (for more detail, see the article by Walshe et al27).

Crash Outcome

Two questions asked whether the participant had ever been involved in a crash as a driver and, if so, how many crashes. The crash history items were recoded into a single binary crash outcome (0 = never; 1 = at least 1 crash).

Statistical Analysis

We examined the Pearson correlation coefficients (r) between the crash outcome and the individual characteristics recorded at wave 6 of the PTS. We used a LGC model computed in Mplus version 8 (Muthén & Muthén) that was sensitive to the binary crash outcome (using the weighted least-squares estimation method) to examine the association between the baseline (intercept) and developmental trajectories (slope) of WM and crashes (Figure 1). Factor scores for reckless driving, driving errors, and the other risk behaviors described were included in the LGC model (model 1). We then estimated a second model (model 2) with the addition of IQ as a test of robustness. The χ2 test statistic, root mean square error of approximation value, comparative fit index, and Tucker-Lewis index were used to determine model fit. Statistical significance was tested at the α = .05 level with 2-tailed tests.

Results
Driver Characteristics and Crashes

Table 1 provides the demographic profile of the sample of young drivers. Four of the driver participants had a self-reported ADHD diagnosis, but none of these individuals reported a crash. Of 84 participants, 39 (46%) were male, and the mean (SD) age was 20.46 (1.09) years. The distribution of sex and ethnicity (measured with investigator-defined responses in the PTS study) closely matches that of the rest of the larger PTS study. However, the driver sample was slightly older at wave 6 (driver mean [SD] age, 18.93 [0.77] years; PTS mean [SD] age, 18.61 [0.62] years; difference, 0.32 years; 95% CI, 0.10-0.53 years; P = .004), with a slightly higher IQ as measured on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (driver mean [SD] score, 103.91 [12.13]; PTS mean [SD] score, 98.05 [15.97]; difference, 5.86; 95% CI, 1.54-10.18; P = .008). Drivers in the sample had a mean (SD) of 3.46 (1.45) years driving experience, started driving at a mean (SD) age of 17 (1.21) years, and drove for a median (interquartile range) of 4.5 (1-10) hours per week. Twenty-five participants (29.8%) reported that they had been involved in at least 1 crash as a driver, with the majority having only 1 crash (18 had 1 crash, 4 had 2 crashes, and 3 had >2 crashes). The crash outcome was not associated with number of years driving (r = 0.21). Many of these young drivers also reported engaging in reckless driving behaviors as captured in the modified Driver Behavior Questionnaire. The most frequently reported reckless behavior was ignoring speed limits (only 32.2% reporting never doing this), and the least reported reckless driving behavior was driving after consuming alcohol (with 85.6% never doing this). Reported crashes were positively associated with reckless driving (r = 0.29; P = .008), evidencing good construct validity for this crash outcome.

Correlation Analysis

Table 2 presents the Philadelphia trajectory study measure scores recorded at wave 6 and Table 3 presents the correlations between the individual characteristics recorded at wave 6, the driving behavior factors, and crash outcome measured at follow-up. While reckless driving was correlated with crashes (r = 0.29), the driving error factor was not (r = −0.18). Crashes did not correlate with age (r = 0.16), sex (r = 0.03), or IQ (r = 0.04). On the other hand, WM at wave 6 was significantly negatively correlated with crashes (r = −0.32; P = .003). No other individual characteristics (ie, behavior, substance use or dependency) were associated with crashes. However, IQ and marijuana use dependence were positively correlated with WM at wave 6, and so alcohol and marijuana dependence were controlled in the models.

Latent Growth Curve Model

We regressed the binary crash outcome on the latent intercept and slope of WM across waves 2 to 6 obtained from the LGC analysis, as well as the factor scores for reckless driving, driving errors, and risk behaviors (model 1). This model provided a good fit to the data (χ227 = 25.877; P = .53; comparative fit index = 1.00; Tucker-Lewis index = 1.02; and root mean square error of approximation = 0.00 [90% CI, 0.00-0.08]). The model indicated that variation in the slope of the WM growth trajectory had a strong independent negative association with crashes (b = −6.41; SE = 2.64; P = .02). In addition, reckless driving was also associated with crashes (b = 0.40; SE = 0.18; P = .03). However, variation in the intercept of WM was not associated with crashes (b = −0.245; SE = 0.67; P = .72), the driving error factor (b = −0.57; SE = 0.33; P = .08), or the risk behavior factor (b = 0.44; SE = 0.31; P = .16) (Figure 1). Among these factors, there was a negative association between the intercept and slope and a positive association between risk behavior and reckless driving; no other factors were significantly correlated. The graph in Figure 2 plots the WM composite scores at each wave, illustrating the relative WM growth trajectories among drivers with and without a crash, as well as nondrivers. Although all 3 groups experienced absolute gains in WM over time, drivers who crashed demonstrated a relatively declining linear trajectory of WM growth that progressively diverged from the other 2 groups, resulting in a larger difference between the groups at wave 6 than at the intercept.

Model 2 included IQ as a possible confounding factor and also provided a good fit to the data (χ232 = 36.556; P = .27; comparative fit index = 0.95; Tucker-Lewis index = 0.93; root mean square error of approximation = 0.04 [90% CI, 0.00-0.09]). Variation in the slope of WM trajectory (b = −7.65; SE = 3.86; P = .047) and reckless driving (b = 0.46; SE = 0.17; P = .008) remained significantly associated with crashes. None of the other variables were significantly associated with crashes, including IQ (b = 0.02; SE = 0.02; P = .33).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first longitudinal study of adolescent WM development in association with motor vehicle crashes. The results demonstrated that the variance in the relative trajectory of WM growth (slope), but not the baseline (intercepts), was associated with crashes within approximately 3 years after the start of driving: adolescents with a divergent trajectory of WM growth had a greater likelihood of being involved in a crash. The relative variance in the growth trajectory of WM remained strongly and independently associated with crashes despite controlling for other risk-related factors, such as reckless driving, drug use, and other risk-related traits and behaviors. Furthermore, the model remained robust with the addition of IQ.

These initial longitudinal results coincide with and expand upon previous cross-sectional studies of association between WM and driving behavior in both self-report and simulated driving studies18-21 and may support the argument that insufficient development of WM at the time of independent driving (rather than a stable individual difference) is critical for identifying young drivers at risk for crashes. As such, the rate of WM development may be an important underlying mechanism of age-graded risk for crashes during adolescent development. However, we do not yet know whether or how WM development may predict crashes and need to further investigate factors that lead to differential trajectories of growth in WM to identify high-risk groups.

Future studies should also investigate the role of WM development in the observed increased risk for unsafe driving and crashes among atypically developing populations (eg, ADHD). The study by Curry et al28 demonstrated that crash risk is 1.36 times higher among adolescents with a diagnosis of ADHD.28 Furthermore, adolescent drivers with ADHD and autism spectrum disorder have more unsafe driving behaviors and errors.29,30 Of note, at wave 6 of this study, WM and crashes did not correlate with impulsivity, suggesting that the association between WM and crashes was not mediated by poor impulse control in this community sample. Previous work has shown an inverse association between WM and impulsivity at earlier ages,23 but impulsivity does not appear to be associated with later adolescent problem behaviors as well as early behaviors.31,32 Future studies with larger sample sizes can better model the effects of impulsivity and substance use, along with WM development, on crash risk. Furthermore, examining clinical populations with impaired impulse control and more severe crash risk may give us insight into how WM development and impulsivity explain crash risk during adolescent development.

Limitations

There are several limitations of this study. First, the crash outcome was self-reported. However, the finding that reckless driving behavior was associated with crashes is consistent with previous work26 and provides some convergent validity for this crash outcome in this study. The crash outcome was not associated with number of years driving, which may be attributed to the fact crash rates are highest within the first year of licensure and our outcome was a cumulative measure of crashes. Second, the sample size may have limited the power to detect associations among the individual characteristic variables (eg, WM development and risk traits and behaviors). Future studies with more power can tease apart the relationships between these factors and the outcome, as well as consider group-based trajectory modeling and latent class analysis to further examine how different developmental trajectories of WM might exert differential influences on crash rates (in both typically and atypically developing adolescents). The association of IQ with WM should also be further examined, with additional factors such as driver training and experience that will change over time with WM development. In addition, while we used a composite score of WM from a battery of performance-based tasks, these measures did not include real-life scenarios or tasks, and so may not be generalizable to how WM would be used during driving. Furthermore, it can be argued that these results could be affected by a practice effect for better learners across waves. However, we do not believe this is likely given that there was at least a year interval between assessments.

Conclusions

This study builds on the scientific foundation for evidence-based guidelines for the counseling and management of young drivers by addressing individual variation in development. While crash statistics indicate that risk is high in young drivers, most young drivers do not crash. Substantial individual variability exists in neural and cognitive development and patterns of risk in adolescence.10,33 Monitoring WM development across adolescence as part of routine assessment could help to identify at-risk drivers, as well as opportunities for intervention. Attention and driving skill deficits due to insufficient WM may be one of the most modifiable risk factors—via experience and skill training. However, we do not yet know how variability in WM contributes to young driver crashes. The advancement of simulated driving technology offers an opportunity to examine how WM development affects skill learning, in-the-moment performance, and risk taking while driving.5 Encouragingly, these findings suggest that any interventions that could improve or enhance WM development during adolescence may offer a novel way to reduce the risk outcomes. By identifying the underlying mechanisms of crashes that span typical and atypical development, we can attempt to develop more broadly applicable interventions across multiple populations.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: July 28, 2019.

Published: September 13, 2019. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.11421

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2019 Walshe EA et al. JAMA Network Open.

Corresponding Author: Elizabeth A. Walshe, PhD, Annenberg Public Policy Center, University of Pennsylvania, 202 S 36th St, Philadelphia, PA 19104 (elizabeth.walshe@appc.upenn.edu).

Author Contributions: Dr Romer had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Winston, Betancourt, Khurana, Romer.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Walshe, Betancourt, Romer.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Walshe, Winston, Khurana, Arena, Romer.

Statistical analysis: Walshe, Romer.

Obtained funding: Betancourt, Khurana, Romer.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Winston, Betancourt, Khurana, Arena.

Supervision: Winston, Romer.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Romer reported receiving grants from the National Institutes of Health during the conduct of the study. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This research was funded by a grant from the US National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01 DA 18913), with support from the Annenberg Public Policy Center at the University of Pennsylvania, as well as the Center for Injury Research and Prevention and Dr Winston’s Distinguished Chair in the Department of Pediatrics at the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References
1.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Leading causes of death reports, national and regional: 1999–2014. http://webappa.cdc.gov/sasweb/ncipc/leadcaus10_us.html. Published 2015. Accessed March 1, 2019.
2.
Tefft  BC; AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. Rates of motor vehicle crashes, injuries and deaths in relation to driver age, United States, 2014-2015. https://aaafoundation.org/rates-motor-vehicle-crashes-injuries-deaths-relation-driver-age-united-states-2014-2015/. Published June 2017. Accessed April 3, 2019.
3.
Curry  AE, Pfeiffer  MR, Durbin  DR, Elliott  MR.  Young driver crash rates by licensing age, driving experience, and license phase.  Accid Anal Prev. 2015;80:243-250. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2015.04.019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Gershon  P, Ehsani  JP, Zhu  C,  et al.  Crash risk and risky driving behavior among adolescents during learner and independent driving periods.  J Adolesc Health. 2018;63(5):568-574. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2018.04.012PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Walshe  EA, Ward McIntosh  C, Romer  D, Winston  FK.  Executive function capacities, negative driving behavior and crashes in young drivers.  Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14(11):1314. doi:10.3390/ijerph14111314PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Cowan  N.  Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education.  Educ Psychol Rev. 2014;26(2):197-223. doi:10.1007/s10648-013-9246-yPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Engle  RW, Kane  MJ.  Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control.  Psychol Learn Motiv. 2004;44:145-200. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44005-XGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Isbell  E, Fukuda  K, Neville  HJ, Vogel  EK.  Visual working memory continues to develop through adolescence.  Front Psychol. 2015;6:696. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00696PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Luna  B, Padmanabhan  A, O’Hearn  K.  What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence?  Brain Cogn. 2010;72(1):101-113. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Satterthwaite  TD, Wolf  DH, Erus  G,  et al.  Functional maturation of the executive system during adolescence.  J Neurosci. 2013;33(41):16249-16261. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2345-13.2013PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Khurana  A, Romer  D, Betancourt  LM, Brodsky  NL, Giannetta  JM, Hurt  H.  Working memory ability predicts trajectories of early alcohol use in adolescents: the mediational role of impulsivity.  Addiction. 2013;108(3):506-515. doi:10.1111/add.12001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Conklin  HM, Luciana  M, Hooper  CJ, Yarger  RS.  Working memory performance in typically developing children and adolescents: behavioral evidence of protracted frontal lobe development.  Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;31(1):103-128. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn3101_6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Baddeley  A.  Working memory: theories, models, and controversies.  Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:1-29. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Diamond  A.  Executive functions.  Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135-168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Groeger  JA.  Understanding Driving: Applying Cognitive Psychology to a Complex Everyday Task. New York, NY: Psychology Press; 2000.
16.
Romer  D, Lee  YC, McDonald  CC, Winston  FK.  Adolescence, attention allocation, and driving safety.  J Adolesc Health. 2014;54(5)(suppl):S6-S15. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.10.202PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Haith  AM, Krakauer  JW.  The multiple effects of practice: skill, habit and reduced cognitive load.  Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018;20:196-201. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Mäntylä  T, Karlsson  MJ, Marklund  M.  Executive control functions in simulated driving.  Appl Neuropsychol. 2009;16(1):11-18. doi:10.1080/09084280802644086PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Pope  CN, Ross  LA, Stavrinos  D.  Association between executive function and problematic adolescent driving.  J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2016;37(9):702-711. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000353PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Starkey  NJ, Isler  RB.  The role of executive function, personality and attitudes to risks in explaining self-reported driving behavior in adolescent and adult male drivers.  Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav. 2016;38:127-136. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2016.01.013Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Ross  V, Jongen  E, Brijs  T, Ruiter  R, Brijs  K, Wets  G.  The relation between cognitive control and risky driving in young novice drivers.  Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2015;22(1):61-72. doi:10.1080/23279095.2013.838958PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Ross  V, Jongen  EM, Wang  W,  et al.  Investigating the influence of working memory capacity when driving behavior is combined with cognitive load: an LCT study of young novice drivers.  Accid Anal Prev. 2014;62:377-387. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2013.06.032PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Romer  D, Betancourt  L, Giannetta  JM, Brodsky  NL, Farah  M, Hurt  H.  Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents.  Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(13):2916-2926. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Khurana  A, Romer  D, Betancourt  LM, Hurt  H.  Modeling trajectories of sensation seeking and impulsivity dimensions from early to late adolescence: universal trends or distinct sub-groups?  J Youth Adolesc. 2018;47(9):1992-2005. doi:10.1007/s10964-018-0891-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Alderman  EM, Johnston  BD; Committee on Adolescence; Council on Injury, Violence, and Poison Prevention.  The teen driver.  Pediatrics. 2018;142(4):e20182163. doi:10.1542/peds.2018-2163PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Reason  J, Manstead  A, Stradling  S, Baxter  J, Campbell  K.  Errors and violations on the roads: a real distinction?  Ergonomics. 1990;33(10-11):1315-1332. doi:10.1080/00140139008925335PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Walshe  EA, Winston  FK, Betancourt  LM, Arena  K, Romer  D.  Cell phone use while driving: a pattern of risk in young novice drivers.  Acad J Pediatr Neonatology. 2018;6(4). doi:10.19080/AJPN.2018.06.555752Google Scholar
28.
Curry  AE, Metzger  KB, Pfeiffer  MR, Elliott  MR, Winston  FK, Power  TJ.  Motor vehicle crash risk among adolescents and young adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  JAMA Pediatr. 2017;171(8):756-763. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.0910PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Classen  S, Monahan  M, Brown  KE, Hernandez  S.  Driving indicators in teens with attention deficit hyperactivity and/or autism spectrum disorder.  Can J Occup Ther. 2013;80(5):274-283. doi:10.1177/0008417413501072PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Fischer  M, Barkley  RA, Smallish  L, Fletcher  K.  Hyperactive children as young adults: driving abilities, safe driving behavior, and adverse driving outcomes.  Accid Anal Prev. 2007;39(1):94-105. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2006.06.008PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Isen  JD, Sparks  JC, Iacono  WG.  Predictive validity of delay discounting behavior in adolescence: a longitudinal twin study.  Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;22(5):434-443. doi:10.1037/a0037340PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
White  HR, Beardslee  J, Pardini  D.  Early predictors of maturing out of marijuana use among young men.  Addict Behav. 2017;65:56-62. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.09.007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Romer  D, Betancourt  LM, Brodsky  NL, Giannetta  JM, Yang  W, Hurt  H.  Does adolescent risk taking imply weak executive function? a prospective study of relations between working memory performance, impulsivity, and risk taking in early adolescence.  Dev Sci. 2011;14(5):1119-1133. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01061.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×