[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Visual Abstract. Candesartan vs Lisinopril for Neurocognitive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment
Candesartan vs Lisinopril for Neurocognitive Function in Older Adults With Mild Cognitive Impairment
Figure 1.  Flow Diagram of Study Recruitment
Flow Diagram of Study Recruitment

AE indicates adverse event; ITT, intention-to-treat; and LTF, loss to follow-up.

aPersonal reasons for withdrawing from candesartan group included: 1 owing to depression, 2 owing to insufficient study compensation to continue, and 1 owing to high home blood pressure readings.

bPersonal reasons for withdrawing from lisinopril group included 1 owing to relocation, 1 owing to busy life schedule, and 1 owing to insufficient study compensation.

cTwo participants discontinued study intervention at 7 months owing to cough and owing to hospitalization for lung tumor resection. The latter was receiving lisinopril as part of their clinical treatment at 12 months. Both participants completed 12-month follow-up visit.

Figure 2.  Mean Sitting and Standing Systolic and Diastolic Blood Pressure (BP) Over Study Period, by Treatment Group
Mean Sitting and Standing Systolic and Diastolic Blood Pressure (BP) Over Study Period, by Treatment Group

P values are obtained from mixed model repeated measure for the interaction of treatment effect over time. Whiskers indicate SDs.

Figure 3.  Change in Trail Making Test Parts B and B − A, and Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised, Delayed Recall and Retention During 12 Months Stratified by Treatment Group
Change in Trail Making Test Parts B and B − A, and Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised, Delayed Recall and Retention During 12 Months Stratified by Treatment Group

Values are model-derived least-square means and SEs (whiskers) from mixed model repeated measure and are adjusted for systolic blood pressure and stratification variables. Trail Making Test analyses are also adjusted for baseline Trail Making Test scores.

P values for the treatment effect are obtained from the mixed model repeated measure models comparing change over the study period (visit as a continuous measure: 1 = baseline, 2 = 6 months, and 3 = 12 months) between the 2 groups: A, P = .004; B, P = .01; C, P = .04; D, P = .02.

Figure 4.  Change in White Matter Lesion Volumes From Baseline to 12 Months Stratified by Treatment Group
Change in White Matter Lesion Volumes From Baseline to 12 Months Stratified by Treatment Group

Adjusted values are least-square means and SEs (whiskers) obtained from mixed model repeated measure and are adjusted for systolic blood pressure, participant race/ethnicity, and prestudy antihypertensive medications.

Table.  Baseline Clinical, Demographic, Cognitive, and Neuroimaging Characteristics Comparisons Between Lisinopril and Candesartan Treatment Groups
Baseline Clinical, Demographic, Cognitive, and Neuroimaging Characteristics Comparisons Between Lisinopril and Candesartan Treatment Groups
1.
Williamson  JD, Pajewski  NM, Auchus  AP,  et al; SPRINT MIND Investigators for the SPRINT Research Group.  Effect of intensive vs standard blood pressure control on probable dementia: a randomized clinical trial.   JAMA. 2019;321(6):553-561. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.21442 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Ma  L, Lu  P, Zhang  Y,  et al.  A15294 Effects of antihypertensive therapy on cognitive function and health-related quality of life in high-risk hypertensive patients.   J Hypertens. 2018;36:e334-e335. doi:10.1097/01.hjh.0000549367.55414.86 Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Gorelick  PB, Nyenhuis  D, Materson  BJ,  et al; American Society of Hypertension Writing Group.  Blood pressure and treatment of persons with hypertension as it relates to cognitive outcomes including executive function.   J Am Soc Hypertens. 2012;6(5):309-315. doi:10.1016/j.jash.2012.08.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Vicario  A, Martinez  CD, Baretto  D, Diaz Casale  A, Nicolosi  L.  Hypertension and cognitive decline: impact on executive function.   J Clin Hypertens (Greenwich). 2005;7(10):598-604. doi:10.1111/j.1524-6175.2005.04498.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Marshall  GA, Rentz  DM, Frey  MT, Locascio  JJ, Johnson  KA, Sperling  RA; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.  Executive function and instrumental activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.   Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):300-308. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2010.04.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Moorhouse  P, Song  X, Rockwood  K,  et al; Consortium to Investigate Vascular Impairment of Cognition.  Executive dysfunction in vascular cognitive impairment in the consortium to investigate vascular impairment of cognition study.   J Neurol Sci. 2010;288(1-2):142-146. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2009.09.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Nasrallah  IM, Pajewski  NM, Auchus  AP,  et al; SPRINT MIND Investigators for the SPRINT Research Group.  Association of intensive vs standard blood pressure control with cerebral white matter lesions.   JAMA. 2019;322(6):524-534. doi:10.1001/jama.2019.10551 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Ahmed  HA, Ishrat  T, Pillai  B,  et al.  RAS modulation prevents progressive cognitive impairment after experimental stroke: a randomized, blinded preclinical trial.   J Neuroinflammation. 2018;15(1):229. doi:10.1186/s12974-018-1262-x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Wharton  W, Goldstein  FC, Zhao  L, Steenland  K, Levey  AI, Hajjar  I.  Modulation of renin-angiotensin system may slow conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63(9):1749-1756. doi:10.1111/jgs.13627 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Li  NC, Lee  A, Whitmer  RA,  et al.  Use of angiotensin receptor blockers and risk of dementia in a predominantly male population: prospective cohort analysis.   BMJ. 2010;340:b5465. doi:10.1136/bmj.b5465 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Zhuang  S, Wang  HF, Li  J, Wang  HY, Wang  X, Xing  CM.  Renin-angiotensin system blockade use and risks of cognitive decline and dementia: A meta-analysis.   Neurosci Lett. 2016;624:53-61. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2016.05.003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Leenen  FHH, Blaustein  MP, Hamlyn  JM.  Update on angiotensin II: new endocrine connections between the brain, adrenal glands and the cardiovascular system.   Endocr Connect. 2017;6(7):R131-R145. doi:10.1530/EC-17-0161 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Basmadjian  OM, Occhieppo  VB, Marchese  NA, Baiardi  G, Bregonzio  C.  Brain angiotensin ii involvement in chronic mental disorders.   Protein Pept Lett. 2017;24(9):817-826. doi:10.2174/0929866524666170728144801 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Das  UN.  Is angiotensin-II an endogenous pro-inflammatory molecule?   Med Sci Monit. 2005;11(5):RA155-RA162.PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Kumaran  D, Udayabanu  M, Kumar  M, Aneja  R, Katyal  A.  Involvement of angiotensin converting enzyme in cerebral hypoperfusion induced anterograde memory impairment and cholinergic dysfunction in rats.   Neuroscience. 2008;155(3):626-639. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.06.023 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Tota  S, Goel  R, Pachauri  SD,  et al.  Effect of angiotensin II on spatial memory, cerebral blood flow, cholinergic neurotransmission, and brain derived neurotrophic factor in rats.   Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;226(2):357-369. doi:10.1007/s00213-012-2913-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Iwai  M, Liu  HW, Chen  R,  et al.  Possible inhibition of focal cerebral ischemia by angiotensin II type 2 receptor stimulation.   Circulation. 2004;110(7):843-848. doi:10.1161/01.CIR.0000138848.58269.80 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Horiuchi  M, Mogi  M.  Role of angiotensin II receptor subtype activation in cognitive function and ischaemic brain damage.   Br J Pharmacol. 2011;163(6):1122-1130. doi:10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.01167.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Rompe  F, Artuc  M, Hallberg  A,  et al.  Direct angiotensin II type 2 receptor stimulation acts anti-inflammatory through epoxyeicosatrienoic acid and inhibition of nuclear factor kappaB.   Hypertension. 2010;55(4):924-931. doi:10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.147843 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Hajjar  I, Hart  M, Chen  YL,  et al.  Antihypertensive therapy and cerebral hemodynamics in executive mild cognitive impairment: results of a pilot randomized clinical trial.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013;61(2):194-201. doi:10.1111/jgs.12100 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
World Medical Association.  World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects.   JAMA. 2013;310(20):2191-2194. doi:10.1001/jama.2013.281053PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Dong  Y, Sharma  VK, Chan  BP,  et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) is superior to the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the detection of vascular cognitive impairment after acute stroke.   J Neurol Sci. 2010;299(1-2):15-18. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2010.08.051 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Nasreddine  ZS, Phillips  NA, Bédirian  V,  et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695-699. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Lobzin  VY.  [Comprehensive early diagnosis of cognitive impairment].  Article in Russian.  Zh Nevrol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova. 2015;115(11):72-79. doi:10.17116/jnevro201511511172-79 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Borella  E, Carretti  B, Mitolo  M,  et al.  Characterizing cognitive inhibitory deficits in mild cognitive impairment.   Psychiatry Res. 2017;251:342-348. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.037 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Kreiner  DS, Ryan  JJ.  Memory and motor skill components of the WAIS-III Digit Symbol-Coding subtest.   Clin Neuropsychol. 2001;15(1):109-113. doi:10.1076/clin.15.1.109.1906 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Ross  TP.  The reliability of cluster and switch scores for the Controlled Oral Word Association Test.   Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2003;18(2):153-164. doi:10.1093/arclin/18.2.153 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Teng  E, Becker  BW, Woo  E, Knopman  DS, Cummings  JL, Lu  PH.  Utility of the functional activities questionnaire for distinguishing mild cognitive impairment from very mild Alzheimer disease.   Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24(4):348-353. doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181e2fc84 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Reitan  RM.  Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage.   Percept Mot Skills. 1958;8(3):271-276. doi:10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271 Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Sánchez-Cubillo  I, Periáñez  JA, Adrover-Roig  D,  et al.  Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(3):438-450. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090626 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Kramer  JH.  Special series introduction: NIH EXAMINER and the assessment of executive functioning.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(1):8-10. doi:10.1017/S1355617713001185 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Kramer  JH, Mungas  D, Possin  KL,  et al.  NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and development of an executive function battery.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(1):11-19. doi:10.1017/S1355617713001094 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Kramer  JH, Mungas  D, Possin  KL,  et al.  NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and development of an executive function battery.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(1):11-19. doi:10.1017/S1355617713001094PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Delis  DC, Freeland  J, Kramer  JH, Kaplan  E.  Integrating clinical assessment with cognitive neuroscience: construct validation of the California Verbal Learning Test.   J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56(1):123-130. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.56.1.123 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Mack  WJ, Freed  DM, Williams  BW, Henderson  VW.  Boston Naming Test: shortened versions for use in Alzheimer’s disease.   J Gerontol. 1992;47(3):154-158. doi:10.1093/geronj/47.3.P154 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Canning  SJ, Leach  L, Stuss  D, Ngo  L, Black  SE.  Diagnostic utility of abbreviated fluency measures in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia.   Neurology. 2004;62(4):556-562. doi:10.1212/WNL.62.4.556 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Wechsler  D.  WMS-R: Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised: Manual. Psychological Corp: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich; 1987.
38.
Radloff  LS.  The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population.   Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385-401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306 Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Lawton  MP, Brody  EM.  Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living.   Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179-186. doi:10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Kamei  S, Hara  M, Serizawa  K,  et al.  Executive dysfunction using behavioral assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome in Parkinson’s disease.   Mov Disord. 2008;23(4):566-573. doi:10.1002/mds.21890 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Vargas  ML, Sanz  JC, Marín  JJ.  Behavioral assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome battery (BADS) in schizophrenia: a pilot study in the Spanish population.   Cogn Behav Neurol. 2009;22(2):95-100. doi:10.1097/WNN.0b013e318192cd08 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Schuit  AJ, Schouten  EG, Westerterp  KR, Saris  WH.  Validity of the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE): according to energy expenditure assessed by the doubly labeled water method.   J Clin Epidemiol. 1997;50(5):541-546. doi:10.1016/S0895-4356(97)00010-3 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Washburn  RA, Smith  KW, Jette  AM, Janney  CA.  The Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE): development and evaluation.   J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46(2):153-162. doi:10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Schmidt  P, Gaser  C, Arsic  M,  et al.  An automated tool for detection of FLAIR-hyperintense white-matter lesions in multiple sclerosis.   Neuroimage. 2012;59(4):3774-3783. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.032 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Moher  D, Hopewell  S, Schulz  KF,  et al.  CONSORT 2010 explanation and elaboration: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials.   BMJ. 2010;340:c869. doi:10.1136/bmj.c869 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hajjar  I, Hart  M, Chen  YL,  et al.  Effect of antihypertensive therapy on cognitive function in early executive cognitive impairment: a double-blind randomized clinical trial.   Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(5):442-444. doi:10.1001/archinternmed.2011.1391 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
O’Brien  PC, Fleming  TR.  A multiple testing procedure for clinical trials.   Biometrics. 1979;35(3):549-556. doi:10.2307/2530245 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Ho  JK, Nation  DA; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.  Memory is preserved in older adults taking AT1 receptor blockers.   Alzheimers Res Ther. 2017;9(1):33. doi:10.1186/s13195-017-0255-9 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Ding  J, Davis-Plourde  KL, Sedaghat  S,  et al.  Antihypertensive medications and risk for incident dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis of individual participant data from prospective cohort studies.   Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(1):61-70. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30393-X PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Anderson  C, Teo  K, Gao  P,  et al; ONTARGET and TRANSCEND Investigators.  Renin-angiotensin system blockade and cognitive function in patients at high risk of cardiovascular disease: analysis of data from the ONTARGET and TRANSCEND studies.   Lancet Neurol. 2011;10(1):43-53. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70250-7 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Trigiani  LJ, Royea  J, Lacalle-Aurioles  M, Tong  XK, Hamel  E.  Pleiotropic benefits of the angiotensin receptor blocker candesartan in a mouse model of Alzheimer disease.   Hypertension. 2018;72(5):1217-1226. doi:10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.118.11775 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Gallinat  S, Yu  M, Dorst  A, Unger  T, Herdegen  T.  Sciatic nerve transection evokes lasting up-regulation of angiotensin AT2 and AT1 receptor mRNA in adult rat dorsal root ganglia and sciatic nerves.   Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1998;57(1):111-122. doi:10.1016/S0169-328X(98)00079-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Schieffer  B, Bünte  C, Witte  J,  et al.  Comparative effects of AT1-antagonism and angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibition on markers of inflammation and platelet aggregation in patients with coronary artery disease.   J Am Coll Cardiol. 2004;44(2):362-368. doi:10.1016/j.jacc.2004.03.065 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Ghiadoni  L, Virdis  A, Magagna  A, Taddei  S, Salvetti  A.  Effect of the angiotensin II type 1 receptor blocker candesartan on endothelial function in patients with essential hypertension.   Hypertension. 2000;35(1 Pt 2):501-506. doi:10.1161/01.HYP.35.1.501 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Oyama  N, Yagita  Y, Sasaki  T,  et al.  An angiotensin II type 1 receptor blocker can preserve endothelial function and attenuate brain ischemic damage in spontaneously hypertensive rats.   J Neurosci Res. 2010;88(13):2889-2898. doi:10.1002/jnr.22441 PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Ando  H, Zhou  J, Macova  M, Imboden  H, Saavedra  JM.  Angiotensin II AT1 receptor blockade reverses pathological hypertrophy and inflammation in brain microvessels of spontaneously hypertensive rats.   Stroke. 2004;35(7):1726-1731. doi:10.1161/01.STR.0000129788.26346.18 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Boyle  PA, Paul  RH, Moser  DJ, Cohen  RA.  Executive impairments predict functional declines in vascular dementia.   Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;18(1):75-82. doi:10.1080/13854040490507172 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
1 Comment for this article
EXPAND ALL
A Ray of Hope?
Arvind Joshi, MBBS MD; FCGP FAMS FICP. | Founder Convener and President: Our Own Discussion Group, Mumbai; Consultant Physician (Medicine and Therapeutics) at Ruchi Diagnostic Center and Ruchi Clinical Laboratory, Kharghar; India.
The findings of this study, especially if strengthened by more studies confirming superiority of candesartan over other angiotensin receptor blockers, may enable the clinicians to fine tune measures to protect elderly hypertensive people from cognitive decline.
It will be interesting to see how various classes of antihypertensive agents as well as various agents within every single class compare with each other with regards to cognition protective effects in particular and neuroprotective effects in general.
It will enrich and deepen the knowledge further if the clinical evaluations are accompanied by laboratory and imaging evaluations.
I congratulate and thank the authors as
well as the participants of this study.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
READ MORE
Original Investigation
Neurology
August 6, 2020

在执行性轻度认知障碍的老年患者中,坎地沙坦与赖诺普利对改善神经认知功能的疗效对比: 随机临床试验

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Neurology, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, Georgia
  • 2Division of General Medicine and Geriatrics, Department of Medicine, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, Georgia
  • 3Division of Cardiology, Department of Medicine, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, Georgia
JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(8):e2012252. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.12252
关键点 español English

问题  对于有高血压和执行性轻度认知障碍的成人患者,血管紧张素受体阻滞剂坎地沙坦和血管紧张素转化酶抑制剂赖诺普利对改善神经认知功能的疗效如何?

结果  这项随机临床试验涉及 176 例高血压成人患者。研究发现,与赖诺普利相比,使用坎地沙坦治疗 12 个月可改善其执行功能和情景记忆。

意义  这些调查结果表明,在高血压和轻度认知障碍的老年患者中,尽管血压水平相当,但相比赖诺普利,使用坎地沙坦疗法仍会提供更好的神经认知疗效。

Abstract

Importance  Observational studies have suggested that angiotensin receptor blockers are associated with a unique cognitive protection. It is unclear if this is due to reduced blood pressure (BP) or angiotensin receptors type 1 blockade.

Objective  To determine neurocognitive effects of candesartan vs lisinopril in older adults with mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

Design, Setting, and Participants  This randomized clinical trial included participants aged 55 years or older with MCI and hypertension. Individuals were withdrawn from prior antihypertensive therapy and randomized in a 1 to 1 ratio to candesartan or lisinopril from June 2014 to December 2018. Participants underwent cognitive assessments at baseline and at 6 and 12 months. Brain magnetic resonance images were obtained at baseline and 12 months. This intent-to-treat study was double-blind and powered for a sample size accounting for 20% dropout. Data were analyzed from May to October 2019.

Interventions  Escalating doses of oral candesartan (up to 32 mg) or lisinopril (up to 40 mg) once daily. Open-label antihypertensive drug treatments were added as needed to achieve BP less than 140/90 mm Hg.

Main Outcomes and Measures  The primary outcome was executive function (measured using the Trail Making Test, Executive Abilities: Measures and Instruments for Neurobehavioral Evaluation and Research tool) and secondary outcomes were episodic memory (measured using the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised) and microvascular brain injury reflected by magnetic resonance images of white matter lesions.

Results  Among 176 randomized participants (mean [SD] age, 66.0 [7.8] years; 101 [57.4%] women; 113 [64.2%] African American), 87 were assigned to candesartan and 89 were assigned to lisinopril. Among these, 141 participants completed the trial, including 77 in the candesartan group and 64 in the lisinopril group. Although the lisinopril vs candesartan groups achieved similar BP (12-month mean [SD] systolic BP: 130 [17] mm Hg vs 134 [20] mm Hg; P = .20; 12-month mean [SD] diastolic BP: 77 [10] mm Hg vs 78 [11] mm Hg; P = .52), candesartan was superior to lisinopril on the primary outcome of executive function measured by Trail Making Test Part B (effect size [ES] = −12.8 [95% CI, −22.5 to −3.1]) but not Executive Abilities: Measures and Instruments for Neurobehavioral Evaluation and Research score (ES = −0.03 [95% CI, −0.08 to 0.03]). Candesartan was also superior to lisinopril on the secondary outcome of Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised delayed recall (ES = 0.4 [95% CI, 0.02 to 0.8]) and retention (ES = 5.1 [95% CI, 0.7 to 9.5]).

Conclusions and Relevance  These findings suggest that in older adults with MCI, 1-year treatment with candesartan had superior neurocognitive outcomes compared with lisinopril. These effects are likely independent of the BP-lowering effect of candesartan.

Trial Registration  ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT01984164

Introduction

Clinical trials have suggested that lowering blood pressure (BP) using antihypertensive therapy provides cognitive protection in individuals without impaired cognition.1,2 It is unclear if this is also true in individuals who have symptoms of cognitive impairment. Studies designed to compare the neurocognitive effects of antihypertensive classes in individuals who are cognitively impaired are limited. It is also unclear if these effects are related to the reduction in BP or to possible pleiotropic BP-independent effects of certain antihypertensive medications. Comparing antihypertensive drug classes independent of the degree of BP lowering is critical to determine if these effects on cognition are only secondary to their BP or other non-BP effects. Such pleiotropic effects of antihypertensive drugs may be leveraged to repurpose drugs for cognitive disorders, including Alzheimer disease (AD).

Another limitation of past studies is the use of a global cognitive measure that may not detect significant effects on executive function. Executive function is a key cognitive domain that is highly susceptible to hypertension,3,4 and impairment in executive function highly correlates with future functional impairment and dementia.5 Executive dysfunction, within the context of hypertension, reflects vascular cognitive impairment and underlying vascular brain injury such as white matter lesions (WMLs) on brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI).6 A 2019 study7 suggested the latter may be ameliorated by antihypertensive therapy. However, it is unclear if there are differential effects among antihypertensive classes.

Prior observational studies have suggested that drugs that modulate the renin angiotensin system (RAS) have a cognitive protective association.8-10 Of the RAS-modulating drugs, angiotensin receptor blockers (ARBs) may have a superior association compared with other classes in affecting cognitive function.11 The potential explanation may be related to their differential blockade of the 2 angiotensin receptors (AT1 and AT2) in the brain.12,13 Activation of AT1 promotes neurotoxic mechanisms, such as oxidative stress, neuroinflammation, endothelial dysfunction, cerebral hypoperfusion and cholinergic depletion.14-16 Stimulation of AT2 counteracts these mechanisms.17-19 Therefore, selective blockade of the AT1 receptors with ARBs may offer superior protection than simultaneously lowering both of AT1 and AT2 receptor activities, such as with angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibitors (ACEIs). In our previous pilot study in adults with hypertension and mild cognitive impairment (MCI),20 candesartan, an ARB, was superior to lisinopril, an ACEI, in providing cognitive protection.

The objective of this 1-year double-blind randomized clinical trial was to compare neurocognitive effects of candesartan with lisinopril in individuals with MCI and hypertension with the goal of achieving equivalent BP levels in both groups.

Methods

The trial protocol and informed consent form were reviewed and approved by the Emory institutional review board committee. All study participants provided written informed consent, and participation was in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.21 For participants who lacked decision capacity based on a study comprehension quiz, a study surrogate or study partner who could provide consent on the participant’s behalf was required. From the initiation of the trial until the final completed analysis, all authors were blinded to the group assignment. An independent biostatistician conducted open interim analyses and presented the study data to a data and safety monitoring board in closed sessions. This study was conducted from June 2014 to December 2018. This report follows the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) reporting guideline.

Trial Population

Eligible participants were 55 years or older with MCI of the executive or mixed type and a history of hypertension defined as systolic BP 140 mm Hg or higher, diastolic BP of 90 mm Hg or higher, or receiving antihypertensive medication. Executive MCI was defined by meeting 3 criteria: (1) Montreal Cognitive Assessment score of at least 2622,23; (2) a performance in at least the 10th percentile on at least 1 of 5 screening tests of executive function, including the Trail Making Test (TMT) Part B,24 modified Stroop Interference Test,25 Digit Span Backward and Digit Sequencing,26 or Verbal Fluency27; and (3) preserved functional ability reflected by a Functional Assessment Questionnaire score of 7 or lower.28

Key exclusion criteria were (1) intolerance to any ACEI or ARB, (2) systolic BP higher than 200 mm Hg or diastolic BP higher than 110 mm Hg, (3) elevated baseline serum creatinine level greater than 1.99 mg/dL (to convert to micromoles per liter, multiply by 1) or serum potassium level greater than 5.5 mEq/dL (to convert to micromoles per liter, multiply by 1), (4) any active medical or psychiatric condition requiring medical attention or deemed a safety risk by the study physician (eg, decompensated heart failure, hematologic disease, stroke in the past 3 years [self-reported clinical stroke], or incidental large vessel infarcts on brain MRI), (5) a confirmed clinical diagnosis of dementia (self-report or caregiver report), and (6) inability to perform study procedures. Participants with contraindications or intolerance to MRI were enrolled but did not undergo neuroimaging. Additional eligibility criteria are described in the Trial Protocol in Supplement 1.

Trial Design

This randomized clinical trial was an investigator-initiated single-center double-blind trial conducted in the metropolitan Atlanta, Georgia, area, comparing the neurocognitive effects of oral candesartan with lisinopril during 1 year. Study participants underwent screening, baseline, 6-month, and 12-month evaluations. Additional visits included biweekly titration visits after randomization to escalate hypertension treatment until goal or maximum drug treatment were achieved. Additional visits with the study physicians were conducted as needed when BP was out of range, adverse events were reported, or at the request of the participant. Visits were supplemented with telephone calls throughout the trial period.

Eligible participants were provided a calibrated BP machine and were trained on using it during the screening visit. All those receiving prestudy antihypertensive medication were guided through a period of gradual taper and washout per a standard protocol. Participants were subsequently randomized in a 1-to-1 block randomization using a computerized random number generator on race (African American vs other) and number of antihypertensive medications prior to study enrollment (≤2 vs >2) to ensure equal distribution between treatment groups and to allow for future preplanned subgroup analyses.

Interventions

Following baseline and randomization, participants received escalating treatment in identical oral capsule formulations and were evaluated every 2 weeks with the goal of achieving mean sitting BP of less than 140/90 mm Hg. All participants commenced the initial daily dosage of candesartan 8 mg or matched capsule of lisinopril 10 mg. Subsequent dose titration for candesartan was 8 mg to 16 mg to 32 mg and for lisinopril was 10 mg to 20 mg to 40 mg. If BP was still higher than 140/90 mm Hg at the highest dose of the blinded medication, additional open-label antihypertensive therapy was added, starting with hydrochlorothiazide at 12.5 mg to 25 mg, amlodipine 2.5 mg to 5 mg to 10 mg, and metoprolol XL 12.5 mg to 25 mg to 50 mg, until BP control was achieved or all classes were used. Participants received all their antihypertensive therapy from the study site for 1 year from the Emory Investigational Drug Services. Throughout the study, participants did not receive any concomitant BP management from other sources, and all nonstudy medications were reviewed at each visit.

Trial Procedures

All participants underwent a full history and examination and provided data by interview or direct observation or examination. Collected data included demographic characteristics (including self-identified race/ethnicity), anthropometric information, seated and standing standardized BP measurements (appropriate cuff size, rest period, unclothed arm), and concurrent medication and supplement inventory. A series of comprehensive neuropsychological tests assessing multiple domains was conducted at baseline, 6 months, and 12 months. Executive function was assessed using TMT Parts A and B.29 Part A was collected to correct for motor speed and visual-perceptual demands on TMT by subtracting completion time for TMT Part A from completion time for Part B (TMT B − A). Part B − A provides a relatively purer measure of executive functioning.30 Participants who could not complete the TMT Part B after 5 minutes were stopped, and their scores were recorded as 300 seconds. A second measure of executive function was collected using the computer-based Executive Abilities: Measures and Instruments for Neurobehavioral Evaluation and Research toolbox.31,32 Each participant was asked to complete a set of tasks using a keyboard and computer screen, and data were further analyzed using item response theory to obtain a single composite executive function score.33 Episodic memory was assessed using the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R),34 which included delayed (ie, 20 minutes) recall, retention, and recognition discrimination indices.34 Language was assessed using the Boston Naming Test.35,36 The Digit Span Test37 and Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression38 scale were also collected. Functional status and levels of independence were measured by the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living scale.39 The Dysexecutive Functioning Questionnaire was collected for evaluating functional abilities related to executive cognitive function.40,41 Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly was used to measure occupational, household, and leisure activities.42,43

All MRI scans were performed using a 3T MRI scanner (Magnetom Prisma; Siemens Healthcare) using a 20-channel head coil at baseline and 12 months. The MRI sequences collected included a 3-dimensional fast spoiled gradient recalled echo sequence, a 3-dimensional T2 fluid-attenuated inversion recovery, fast spin echo sequence and arterial spin labeling. The volumetric measures of the hippocampus and other cortical/subcortical regions were extracted using FreeSurfer software version 5.3 (FreeSurfer) from T1MPRAGE structural image. The fluid-attenuated inversion recovery images were used to calculate WML volume using the Lesion Segmentation Tool in Statistical Parametric Mapping.44 Details of image acquisition and processing are in the Trial Protocol in Supplement 1.

Safety

Safety assessments included a physical and neurological examination prior to randomization, periodic assessment of medical history data and clinical measures (eg, sitting and standing BP, heart rate, routine laboratory measurements), and surveillance of adverse events. Significant events identified during the study period through self-report by the participant or next of kin, or clinically significant laboratory results outside of reference ranges were recorded as adverse events. Participants were screened for a new diagnosis of dementia or the use of cholinesterase inhibitors or memantine at each visit.

Outcomes

The primary outcome for this trial was executive function (ie, TMT and Executive Abilities: Measures and Instruments for Neurobehavioral Evaluation and Research executive composite score) measured 3 times: baseline, 6 months, and 12 months. The secondary outcomes included episodic memory (ie, HVLT-R delayed recall, retention, and recognition discrimination), other cognitive domains (ie, language using the Boston Naming Test and attention using the Digit Span Test) and brain MRI (ie, WML volume, hippocampal volume and global cerebral perfusion).

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted using the intention-to-treat principle. Baseline characteristics were summarized by randomization group using descriptive statistics including mean (SD) and count (percentage). We performed χ2 test on categorical data, and t test or Wilcoxon rank test were performed on continuous data. The primary analysis was performed using mixed model with repeated measures. The treatment group was the primary factor of interest. Least-square means were derived from these models. We included the stratification variables (ie, race/ethnicity and number of antihypertensive medications prior to study enrollment) and systolic BP (mean of 2 seated readings as a repeated measure in the model) to assess the drug effects adjusted for systolic BP changes during the study period. Sensitivity analyses were conducted by adjusting for the baseline outcome measures for those that differed at baseline between the 2 groups. Results are provided as adjusted least-square means with SE and effect size (ES) estimates (calculated from mixed model with repeated measures as the adjusted difference in cognitive change between the 2 groups over the study period). Time was included as a continuous variable (with T1 indicating baseline; T2, 6 months; and T3, 12 months) with 2-sided 95% CIs as recommended by the updated CONSORT guidelines for continuous outcomes.45 Additional details are provided in the Trial Protocol in Supplement 1.

Using a 2-sided α = .05, an estimated sample size of 140 would provide 80% power to detect at least an ES of 0.24 on executive function (TMT Part B − A) in our pilot study.20,46 To account for a dropout rate of 20%, we preplanned to enroll approximately 175 participants. Interim analysis with the data and safety monitoring board was completed by an independent statistical team when 50% of the trial sample finished 12-month visits. We used an α spending function approach with O’Brien-Fleming sequential boundaries (α levels at .0056 and .0482) to preserve the overall 2-sided α error rate of .05.47 The data and safety monitoring board recommended continuation and completion of the study after the interim analysis. Statistical analyses were performed with SAS statistical software version 9.4 (SAS Institute). Data were analyzed from May to October 2019.

Results

Of 377 individuals screened, 176 (47%) were eligible for the study, agreed to proceed with tapering of previously prescribed antihypertensive therapy, and were randomized to candesartan or lisinopril. Of those randomized, 33 dropped out before the 6-month assessment, and an additional 2 participants dropped out after the 6-month assessment. Two participants completed the study but had their intervention discontinued after 6 months owing to hospitalization for resection of a lung mass or a persistent cough. The final sample for the intention-to-treat analysis was 141 individuals, including 77 in the candesartan group and 64 in the lisinopril group. Figure 1 shows the CONSORT flowchart for this trial.

Baseline Characteristics

Among 176 randomized participants, including 87 assigned to candesartan and 89 assigned to lisinopril, the mean (SD) age was 66.0 (7.8) years 101 (57.4%) were women, and 113 were African American (64.2%). Baseline group comparisons are shown in the Table. There was no difference between the 2 groups in the qualifying task for executive dysfunction during the screening process. Participants assigned to either lisinopril or candesartan were also similar on all baseline characteristics, including cognitive performance, except for the baseline TMT Part B (mean [SD] score, 130.6 [78.7] seconds vs 156.0 [8.0] seconds) and Part B − A (mean [SD] score, 92.3 [74.0] seconds vs 115.4 [79.4] seconds). When we divided the group into those who scored 300 seconds on the TMT Part B at baseline (the maximum allowable time to finish the task), there was no difference in the number of participants who scored 300 seconds between the lisinopril group vs candesartan group (12 individuals [13.5%] vs 18 individuals [20.7%]). The mean (SD) TMT Part B score in those with a score less than 300 seconds was 104.2 (43.9) seconds in the lisinopril group vs 118.4 (49.4) seconds in the candesartan group, and the mean (SD) score for TMT Part B − A was 67.3 (58.2) seconds in the lisinopril group vs 80.8 (44.7) seconds in the candesartan group (P = .06). The TMT analyses were adjusted for baseline performances.

Among individuals randomized to candesartan, 44 individuals (50.6%) had mixed executive and amnestic-type MCI vs 33 individuals (37.1%) in the lisinopril group (P = .07). There was no difference in pre-enrollment use of ARBs or ACEIs by randomization group. There was also no difference between those who completed the study and those who dropped out before the 12-month assessment (eTable 1 in Supplement 2). There was no difference in the number of participants who completed vs dropped out of the candesartan or lisinopril treatment groups.

Intervention Descriptions and BP

Nearly half of the study sample (86 participants [48.9%]) achieved BP control on the initial dose of candesartan (8 mg) or lisinopril (10 mg), and 46 participants (26.1%) needed an additional agent after maximal titration of study medications. The distribution of candesartan or lisinopril doses and of the additional open-label antihypertensive medication doses were similar between the 2 groups (eTable 2 in Supplement 2). Both groups achieved equivalent BPs during the study with no difference in either sitting or standing systolic and diastolic BP at baseline (ie, without any treatment) or during the follow-up period. At the 12-month visit, mean (SD) systolic BPs were 130 (17) mm Hg for lisinopril vs 134 (11) mm Hg for candesartan (P = .20), and mean (SD) diastolic BPs were 77 (10) mm Hg for lisinopril vs 78 (11) mm Hg for candesartan (P = .52). Similarly, serum potassium and creatinine levels did not differ between the 2 groups. These results are shown in Figure 2 and eTable 3 and the eFigure in Supplement 2.

Adverse Events

More participants reported cough in the lisinopril group compared with the candesartan group (24 participants [27.0%] vs 7 participants [8.0%]; P = .005), but since both groups experienced cough, this did not lead to unblinding of the study team. The candesartan group reported more skin rash than the lisinopril group (8 participants [9.1%] vs 1 participant [1.1%]; P = .008). There were no differences in other adverse events reported during the study (eTable 4 in Supplement 2). More participants in the lisinopril group withdrew from the study owing to adverse events compared with the candesartan group (25 participants [28.1%] vs 10 participants [11.5%]; P = .01) (Figure 1).

Cognitive Outcomes

After adjusting for systolic BP and stratification variables, participants randomized to candesartan demonstrated a significant improvement in executive function compared with those randomized to lisinopril, who showed decline, measured by TMT Part B (ES = −12.0 [95% CI, −21.7 to −2.3]) seconds; P = .01) and TMT Part B − A (ES = −13.6 [95% CI, −23.6 to −3.7] seconds; P = .008). Since TMT Part B was different between groups at baseline, further analysis was conducted to adjust for baseline performance on this task. The baseline-adjusted analyses showed similar results (TMT Part B: ES = −12.8 [95% CI, −22.6 to −3.1] seconds; P = .01; TMT Part B − A: ES = −14.3 [95% CI, −24.2 to −4.5]; P = .004). At the 12-month visit, the adjusted TMT Part B and B − A were significantly better in the candesartan group compared with the lisinopril group (TMT Part B: mean [SE] score, 128.3 [5.4] seconds vs 150.2 [5.9] seconds; difference, 22.3 [95% CI, 7.3 to 37.3] seconds; P = .004; TMT Part B − A: mean [SE] score, 87.2 [5.5] seconds vs 111.4 [5.9] seconds; difference, 24.6 [95% CI, 9.5 to 39.7] seconds). There were no differences between treatment groups in Executive Abilities: Measures and Instruments for Neurobehavioral Evaluation and Research scores, our second executive function measure (ES = −0.03 [95% CI, −0.08 to 0.03] seconds; P = .31) (Figure 3A and B; eTable 5 in Supplement 2).

Treatment with candesartan had a superior effect on episodic memory, measured by the delayed recall and retention indices of the HVLT-R compared with lisinopril after adjusting for systolic BP and stratification variables. The ES for delayed recall was 0.4 (95% CI, 0.02 to 0.8; P = .04), and the ES for retention was 5.1 (95% CI, 0.7 to 9.5; P = .02) (Figure 3C and D); eTable 5 in Supplement 2). We did not observe group differences in the additional secondary cognitive outcomes of the Boston Naming Test and Digit Span Tests or in the functional measures of Instrumental Activities of Daily Living or Dysexecutive Functioning Questionnaire (eTable 5 in Supplement 2).

MRI Outcomes

Of 176 randomized participants, 27 did not complete an MRI scan at baseline (15 had metal implants with either pacemaker, stent, or inferior vena cava filter and 12 were claustrophobic). Of 141 participants who returned for their 12-month visit, an additional 8 participants refused to have a follow-up MRI. The final sample with both baseline and 12-month MRI was 104 participants (73.8%), including 51 participants in the candesartan group and 53 participants in the lisinopril group.

At baseline, the lisinopril group had a mean (SD) WML volume of 4.8 (8.4) mm3, and the candesartan group had a mean (SD) of 2.3 (2.3) mm3. After adjusting for BP and stratification variables, those randomized to candesartan had less WML accumulation of 0.2 (95% CI, −0.2 to 0.7) mm3 vs 0.8 (95% CI, 0.3 to 1.4) mm3 in the lisinopril group (ES = −0.3 [95% CI, −0.6 to 0]). Correcting for baseline levels did not alter the results significantly (change in candesartan group: 0.1 [95% CI, −0.3 to 0.5] mm3; change in the lisinopril group: 0.6 [95% CI, 0.2 to 1.0] mm3; ES = −0.2 [95% CI, −0.5 to 0]). The results of WML change are shown in Figure 4. There was no significant difference in hippocampal volume (although both groups showed a significant loss of hippocampal volume) or cerebral perfusion by treatment group (eTable 5 in Supplement 2).

Discussion

In this randomized clinical trial of individuals with MCI and hypertension, 1-year therapy with candesartan was associated with better cognitive outcomes compared with lisinopril, including 1 of the 2 executive function measures, and in memory. Those randomized to lisinopril had significant accumulation of WML, whereas those randomized to candesartan had no significant increase in WML. These effects were independent of BP-lowering properties.

In the largest observational study to our knowledge, use of ARBs was associated with lower risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease (AD) compared with ACEIs and other antihypertensives.10 Analysis of the Alzheimer Disease Neuroimaging Initiative data48 suggests that people who use ARBs have better cognitive trajectories over 3 years and less WML accumulation. More recently, Ding and colleagues,49 in a participant-level meta-analysis of cohort observational studies of predominantly White individuals, reported no differences in antihypertensive classes and the risk of dementia or AD. Although no specific comparisons between ACEI and ARB use was presented, that study did not seem to account for BP change over a long period. The hypertension group excluded those on treatment with controlled BP and did not investigate the association with executive function. This study, in contrast, suggests that when the BP effects are matched in a clinical trial, ARBs provide neurocognitive protection on executive function even over the short-term. This is the second clinical trial in hypertension and MCI suggesting these favorable effects.46 However, in this trial we also observed that candesartan may positively affect memory, which suggests a role in amnestic disorders, such as AD.

Prior trials have suggested that lowering of BP is protective against cognitive decline. Only a few of these were designed to compare drug regimens and most excluded individuals with symptomatic cognitive impairment. Two large trials comparing ACEI and ARB included cognitive outcomes: Ongoing Telmisartan Alone and in Combination With Ramipril Global Endpoint Trial, which compared ACEI, ARB, and the combination, and Telmisartan Randomized Assessment Study in ACE Intolerant Subjects With Cardiovascular Disease trial, which compared a placebo with ARB in participants who were intolerant to ACEIs.50 Neither of these studies included cognitively symptomatic individuals, and the cognitive measure was a single test of global cognition, the Mini-Mental State Exam, which is rather insensitive to executive impairment and in detecting change over time. Hence, our study addresses a key gap in the literature. Although further larger and longer-term studies are needed, this randomized clinical trial provides additional and incremental support to the positive neurocognitive effects of candesartan that are likely independent and additive to their BP-lowering effects.

Based on our current understanding of the brain RAS activity, we propose that the neurocognitive benefits of candesartan are due to the selectivity of the blockade of the AT1 receptor, allowing the AT2 receptor to remain active. Supporting evidence for AT2 receptor activation comes from animal models in which AT2 receptor agonists decreased brain infarct area after ischemic injury by increasing cerebral perfusion in the penumbra,17 decreased superoxide production,17 and decreased inflammation and axonal degeneration.19,51 Activation of the AT2 receptor is associated with axonal regeneration, neuronal repair,52 and decreases in vascular inflammation.19 Additionally, ARBs reduce inflammatory markers more than ACEIs53 and potentially restore endothelial dysfunction via AT2 receptor stimulation.54-56 These potential mechanisms should be the focus of future investigations and analyses of this trial data.

Executive function is an important domain that is highly susceptible to vascular brain injury. Hypertension and executive impairment are associated with microvascular brain damage, and our study provides initial evidence that candesartan, but not lisinopril, protects against WML accumulation and vascular brain injury. However, it is worth noting that the between group difference was not statistically significant.

The advantages of this study are the inclusion of participants with MCI and comparing 2 drugs that modulate RAS differently, hence providing important insights into the role of RAS in the brain in individuals who are cognitively impaired. Evaluating measures of executive function, the domain most likely to be affected by hypertension and microvascular disease, provides additional unique insights into the hypertension-cognition-RAS axis. In addition, we recruited a highly diverse population and achieved similar BP levels in both treatment groups.

Limitations

A limitation of this study is its 1-year study period. Although we have found a positive effect on individual cognitive domains, translation into dementia care should be interpreted within that short-term context. Although executive function has been identified as risk factor for future functional decline, our study did not demonstrate an effect on our functional measures (ie, Dysexecutive Functioning Questionnaire or Instrumental Activities of Daily Living).57 This is likely owing to the duration of the study combined with a possible ceiling effect on these measures, since our sample was functionally independent at baseline. Longer study duration may offer greater insight into the functional effects of candesartan. Further, this study cannot be generalizable to other drugs within the same classes of ARB and ACEI, and our findings are applicable to a single drug effect within a class, and not a class effect. Additional limitations include the relatively small sample size, which limited our power to correct for multiple comparisons. Approximately 25% of the clinical trial sample did not have neuroimaging data. Although we were able to detect a possible difference in microvascular disease, our ability to examine the effect on hippocampal volume or cerebral perfusion is likely limited by the power of the neuroimaging component and the 1-year duration of the study. Although we have provided analytical approaches to account for the observed differences at baseline in TMT Part B and WML, our results should be interpreted within the context of this limitation. Nevertheless, our study provides additional evidence for the potential role of ARBs in neurocognitive protection. While these findings would fall into hypotheses generation and justify larger trials, ARBs in general, and candesartan in particular, offer an intriguing therapeutic possibility for cognitive disorders in relation to vascular brain injury, and especially when considered cumulatively with prior observational studies10 and our prior pilot.20 Currently, there are no available drugs for executive MCI or other cognitive disorders that are becoming more prevalent as populations age. Hence, further investigations of ARBs in this area are critical.

Conclusions

The results of this randomized clinical trial demonstrate that candesartan is superior to lisinopril on executive function and memory outcomes in MCI. These effects are independent of but likely additive to their BP-lowering effects. Future studies in executive and memory impairments, such as those with prodromal AD, are warranted.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: May 20, 2020.

Published: August 6, 2020. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.12252

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2020 Hajjar I et al. JAMA Network Open.

Corresponding Author: Ihab Hajjar, MD, MS, Department of Neurology, Department of Medicine, Emory University School of Medicine, 6 Executive Park Dr NE, Second Floor, Atlanta, GA 30329 (ihabhajjar@emory.edu).

Author Contributions: Mr McDaniel and Dr Hajjar had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Hajjar, Levey.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Hajjar, Okafor, McDaniel, Obideen, Dee, Shokouhi, Quyyumi, Goldstein.

Drafting of the manuscript: Hajjar, Okafor, McDaniel, Obideen, Dee, Shokouhi.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Hajjar, Okafor, McDaniel, Quyyumi, Levey, Goldstein.

Statistical analysis: Hajjar, McDaniel, Obideen, Shokouhi.

Obtained funding: Hajjar, Quyyumi.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Hajjar, Okafor, Obideen, Dee, Levey.

Supervision: Hajjar, Okafor, Dee.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Levey reported receiving personal fees from Karuna Pharmaceuticals and GENUV and grants from Cognito Therapeutics, vTV Therapeutics, Abbvie, Biogen, Esai, Genentech, and Novartis outside the submitted work. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by grant No. R01 AG042127 from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (Dr Hajjar). Antihypertensive study drugs were purchased by and dispensed through Emory University Investigational Drug Services and supported by funds from the NIH grant.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funder had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 3.

Additional Contributions: Peter Novak, MD, PhD (Harvard Medical School); Ali Ahmed, MD (Washington DC VA Hospital); Oscar Lopez, MD (University of Pittsburg); and Inmaculada Aban, PhD (University of Alabama at Birmingham), served as the data safety monitoring board. John Hanfelt, PhD, and Neeta Shenvi, MS (Rollins School of Public Health, Emory University), served as the unblinded biostatistical team. These individuals were compensated for their contributions from the NIH grant.

References
1.
Williamson  JD, Pajewski  NM, Auchus  AP,  et al; SPRINT MIND Investigators for the SPRINT Research Group.  Effect of intensive vs standard blood pressure control on probable dementia: a randomized clinical trial.   JAMA. 2019;321(6):553-561. doi:10.1001/jama.2018.21442 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Ma  L, Lu  P, Zhang  Y,  et al.  A15294 Effects of antihypertensive therapy on cognitive function and health-related quality of life in high-risk hypertensive patients.   J Hypertens. 2018;36:e334-e335. doi:10.1097/01.hjh.0000549367.55414.86 Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Gorelick  PB, Nyenhuis  D, Materson  BJ,  et al; American Society of Hypertension Writing Group.  Blood pressure and treatment of persons with hypertension as it relates to cognitive outcomes including executive function.   J Am Soc Hypertens. 2012;6(5):309-315. doi:10.1016/j.jash.2012.08.004 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Vicario  A, Martinez  CD, Baretto  D, Diaz Casale  A, Nicolosi  L.  Hypertension and cognitive decline: impact on executive function.   J Clin Hypertens (Greenwich). 2005;7(10):598-604. doi:10.1111/j.1524-6175.2005.04498.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Marshall  GA, Rentz  DM, Frey  MT, Locascio  JJ, Johnson  KA, Sperling  RA; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.  Executive function and instrumental activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease.   Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):300-308. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2010.04.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Moorhouse  P, Song  X, Rockwood  K,  et al; Consortium to Investigate Vascular Impairment of Cognition.  Executive dysfunction in vascular cognitive impairment in the consortium to investigate vascular impairment of cognition study.   J Neurol Sci. 2010;288(1-2):142-146. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2009.09.017 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Nasrallah  IM, Pajewski  NM, Auchus  AP,  et al; SPRINT MIND Investigators for the SPRINT Research Group.  Association of intensive vs standard blood pressure control with cerebral white matter lesions.   JAMA. 2019;322(6):524-534. doi:10.1001/jama.2019.10551 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Ahmed  HA, Ishrat  T, Pillai  B,  et al.  RAS modulation prevents progressive cognitive impairment after experimental stroke: a randomized, blinded preclinical trial.   J Neuroinflammation. 2018;15(1):229. doi:10.1186/s12974-018-1262-x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Wharton  W, Goldstein  FC, Zhao  L, Steenland  K, Levey  AI, Hajjar  I.  Modulation of renin-angiotensin system may slow conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015;63(9):1749-1756. doi:10.1111/jgs.13627 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Li  NC, Lee  A, Whitmer  RA,  et al.  Use of angiotensin receptor blockers and risk of dementia in a predominantly male population: prospective cohort analysis.   BMJ. 2010;340:b5465. doi:10.1136/bmj.b5465 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Zhuang  S, Wang  HF, Li  J, Wang  HY, Wang  X, Xing  CM.  Renin-angiotensin system blockade use and risks of cognitive decline and dementia: A meta-analysis.   Neurosci Lett. 2016;624:53-61. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2016.05.003 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Leenen  FHH, Blaustein  MP, Hamlyn  JM.  Update on angiotensin II: new endocrine connections between the brain, adrenal glands and the cardiovascular system.   Endocr Connect. 2017;6(7):R131-R145. doi:10.1530/EC-17-0161 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Basmadjian  OM, Occhieppo  VB, Marchese  NA, Baiardi  G, Bregonzio  C.  Brain angiotensin ii involvement in chronic mental disorders.   Protein Pept Lett. 2017;24(9):817-826. doi:10.2174/0929866524666170728144801 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Das  UN.  Is angiotensin-II an endogenous pro-inflammatory molecule?   Med Sci Monit. 2005;11(5):RA155-RA162.PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Kumaran  D, Udayabanu  M, Kumar  M, Aneja  R, Katyal  A.  Involvement of angiotensin converting enzyme in cerebral hypoperfusion induced anterograde memory impairment and cholinergic dysfunction in rats.   Neuroscience. 2008;155(3):626-639. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.06.023 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Tota  S, Goel  R, Pachauri  SD,  et al.  Effect of angiotensin II on spatial memory, cerebral blood flow, cholinergic neurotransmission, and brain derived neurotrophic factor in rats.   Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;226(2):357-369. doi:10.1007/s00213-012-2913-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Iwai  M, Liu  HW, Chen  R,  et al.  Possible inhibition of focal cerebral ischemia by angiotensin II type 2 receptor stimulation.   Circulation. 2004;110(7):843-848. doi:10.1161/01.CIR.0000138848.58269.80 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Horiuchi  M, Mogi  M.  Role of angiotensin II receptor subtype activation in cognitive function and ischaemic brain damage.   Br J Pharmacol. 2011;163(6):1122-1130. doi:10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.01167.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Rompe  F, Artuc  M, Hallberg  A,  et al.  Direct angiotensin II type 2 receptor stimulation acts anti-inflammatory through epoxyeicosatrienoic acid and inhibition of nuclear factor kappaB.   Hypertension. 2010;55(4):924-931. doi:10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.109.147843 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Hajjar  I, Hart  M, Chen  YL,  et al.  Antihypertensive therapy and cerebral hemodynamics in executive mild cognitive impairment: results of a pilot randomized clinical trial.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013;61(2):194-201. doi:10.1111/jgs.12100 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
World Medical Association.  World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects.   JAMA. 2013;310(20):2191-2194. doi:10.1001/jama.2013.281053PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Dong  Y, Sharma  VK, Chan  BP,  et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) is superior to the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the detection of vascular cognitive impairment after acute stroke.   J Neurol Sci. 2010;299(1-2):15-18. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2010.08.051 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Nasreddine  ZS, Phillips  NA, Bédirian  V,  et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment.   J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695-699. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Lobzin  VY.  [Comprehensive early diagnosis of cognitive impairment].  Article in Russian.  Zh Nevrol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova. 2015;115(11):72-79. doi:10.17116/jnevro201511511172-79 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Borella  E, Carretti  B, Mitolo  M,  et al.  Characterizing cognitive inhibitory deficits in mild cognitive impairment.   Psychiatry Res. 2017;251:342-348. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.037 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Kreiner  DS, Ryan  JJ.  Memory and motor skill components of the WAIS-III Digit Symbol-Coding subtest.   Clin Neuropsychol. 2001;15(1):109-113. doi:10.1076/clin.15.1.109.1906 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Ross  TP.  The reliability of cluster and switch scores for the Controlled Oral Word Association Test.   Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2003;18(2):153-164. doi:10.1093/arclin/18.2.153 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Teng  E, Becker  BW, Woo  E, Knopman  DS, Cummings  JL, Lu  PH.  Utility of the functional activities questionnaire for distinguishing mild cognitive impairment from very mild Alzheimer disease.   Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010;24(4):348-353. doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181e2fc84 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Reitan  RM.  Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage.   Percept Mot Skills. 1958;8(3):271-276. doi:10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271 Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Sánchez-Cubillo  I, Periáñez  JA, Adrover-Roig  D,  et al.  Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009;15(3):438-450. doi:10.1017/S1355617709090626 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Kramer  JH.  Special series introduction: NIH EXAMINER and the assessment of executive functioning.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(1):8-10. doi:10.1017/S1355617713001185 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Kramer  JH, Mungas  D, Possin  KL,  et al.  NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and development of an executive function battery.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(1):11-19. doi:10.1017/S1355617713001094 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Kramer  JH, Mungas  D, Possin  KL,  et al.  NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and development of an executive function battery.   J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2014;20(1):11-19. doi:10.1017/S1355617713001094PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Delis  DC, Freeland  J, Kramer  JH, Kaplan  E.  Integrating clinical assessment with cognitive neuroscience: construct validation of the California Verbal Learning Test.   J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56(1):123-130. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.56.1.123 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Mack  WJ, Freed  DM, Williams  BW, Henderson  VW.  Boston Naming Test: shortened versions for use in Alzheimer’s disease.   J Gerontol. 1992;47(3):154-158. doi:10.1093/geronj/47.3.P154 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Canning  SJ, Leach  L, Stuss  D, Ngo  L, Black  SE.  Diagnostic utility of abbreviated fluency measures in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia.   Neurology. 2004;62(4):556-562. doi:10.1212/WNL.62.4.556 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Wechsler  D.  WMS-R: Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised: Manual. Psychological Corp: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich; 1987.
38.
Radloff  LS.  The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population.   Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385-401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306 Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Lawton  MP, Brody  EM.  Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living.   Gerontologist. 1969;9(3):179-186. doi:10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Kamei  S, Hara  M, Serizawa  K,  et al.  Executive dysfunction using behavioral assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome in Parkinson’s disease.   Mov Disord. 2008;23(4):566-573. doi:10.1002/mds.21890 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Vargas  ML, Sanz  JC, Marín  JJ.  Behavioral assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome battery (BADS) in schizophrenia: a pilot study in the Spanish population.   Cogn Behav Neurol. 2009;22(2):95-100. doi:10.1097/WNN.0b013e318192cd08 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Schuit  AJ, Schouten  EG, Westerterp  KR, Saris  WH.  Validity of the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE): according to energy expenditure assessed by the doubly labeled water method.   J Clin Epidemiol. 1997;50(5):541-546. doi:10.1016/S0895-4356(97)00010-3 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Washburn  RA, Smith  KW, Jette  AM, Janney  CA.  The Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE): development and evaluation.   J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46(2):153-162. doi:10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Schmidt  P, Gaser  C, Arsic  M,  et al.  An automated tool for detection of FLAIR-hyperintense white-matter lesions in multiple sclerosis.   Neuroimage. 2012;59(4):3774-3783. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.032 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Moher  D, Hopewell  S, Schulz  KF,  et al.  CONSORT 2010 explanation and elaboration: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials.   BMJ. 2010;340:c869. doi:10.1136/bmj.c869 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hajjar  I, Hart  M, Chen  YL,  et al.  Effect of antihypertensive therapy on cognitive function in early executive cognitive impairment: a double-blind randomized clinical trial.   Arch Intern Med. 2012;172(5):442-444. doi:10.1001/archinternmed.2011.1391 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
O’Brien  PC, Fleming  TR.  A multiple testing procedure for clinical trials.   Biometrics. 1979;35(3):549-556. doi:10.2307/2530245 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Ho  JK, Nation  DA; Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative.  Memory is preserved in older adults taking AT1 receptor blockers.   Alzheimers Res Ther. 2017;9(1):33. doi:10.1186/s13195-017-0255-9 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Ding  J, Davis-Plourde  KL, Sedaghat  S,  et al.  Antihypertensive medications and risk for incident dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a meta-analysis of individual participant data from prospective cohort studies.   Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(1):61-70. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(19)30393-X PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Anderson  C, Teo  K, Gao  P,  et al; ONTARGET and TRANSCEND Investigators.  Renin-angiotensin system blockade and cognitive function in patients at high risk of cardiovascular disease: analysis of data from the ONTARGET and TRANSCEND studies.   Lancet Neurol. 2011;10(1):43-53. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70250-7 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Trigiani  LJ, Royea  J, Lacalle-Aurioles  M, Tong  XK, Hamel  E.  Pleiotropic benefits of the angiotensin receptor blocker candesartan in a mouse model of Alzheimer disease.   Hypertension. 2018;72(5):1217-1226. doi:10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.118.11775 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Gallinat  S, Yu  M, Dorst  A, Unger  T, Herdegen  T.  Sciatic nerve transection evokes lasting up-regulation of angiotensin AT2 and AT1 receptor mRNA in adult rat dorsal root ganglia and sciatic nerves.   Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1998;57(1):111-122. doi:10.1016/S0169-328X(98)00079-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Schieffer  B, Bünte  C, Witte  J,  et al.  Comparative effects of AT1-antagonism and angiotensin-converting enzyme inhibition on markers of inflammation and platelet aggregation in patients with coronary artery disease.   J Am Coll Cardiol. 2004;44(2):362-368. doi:10.1016/j.jacc.2004.03.065 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Ghiadoni  L, Virdis  A, Magagna  A, Taddei  S, Salvetti  A.  Effect of the angiotensin II type 1 receptor blocker candesartan on endothelial function in patients with essential hypertension.   Hypertension. 2000;35(1 Pt 2):501-506. doi:10.1161/01.HYP.35.1.501 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Oyama  N, Yagita  Y, Sasaki  T,  et al.  An angiotensin II type 1 receptor blocker can preserve endothelial function and attenuate brain ischemic damage in spontaneously hypertensive rats.   J Neurosci Res. 2010;88(13):2889-2898. doi:10.1002/jnr.22441 PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Ando  H, Zhou  J, Macova  M, Imboden  H, Saavedra  JM.  Angiotensin II AT1 receptor blockade reverses pathological hypertrophy and inflammation in brain microvessels of spontaneously hypertensive rats.   Stroke. 2004;35(7):1726-1731. doi:10.1161/01.STR.0000129788.26346.18 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Boyle  PA, Paul  RH, Moser  DJ, Cohen  RA.  Executive impairments predict functional declines in vascular dementia.   Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;18(1):75-82. doi:10.1080/13854040490507172 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×